Today the UK government Department of Health published its final report on the Winterbourne View care scandal. A joint project between Bangor University and Mencap ("Who's Challenging Who") is described in the Good Practice Examples accompanying report. The Good Practice report, along with the full review report and Easy Read versions can be downloaded for free at:
http://www.dh.gov.uk/health/2012/12/final-winterbourne/
The summary of the research evaluation of Who's Challenging Who is now in press with the Journal of Intellectual Disability Research:
http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1365-2788.2012.01630.x/abstract
The Department of Health Good Practice Examples report includes the following information about Who's Challenging Who (quoted from page 80 of the report):
"The Who’s Challenging Who project
Who’s Challenging Who (WCW) is an attitude change training package that has been developed over 18 months since January 2011 (pre‐dating Winterbourne View). The development and pilot evaluation of WCW was funded by a Knowledge Transfer Partnership grant between Mencap and Bangor University.
WCW involves a person with learning disability who has behaviour that challenges working with a person without disability as co‐trainers for a half day workshop targeting health and social care support staff.
The WCW training curriculum was informed jointly by the experiences of the co-trainers with learning disability and by existing research evidence. The theory behind the WCW training is to provide multiple opportunities for direct contact with individuals whose behaviour challenges and contact with information about their experiences of services and being labelled as “challenging”. Small groups of support staff (up to 10) experience an intensive interactive session with the aim of increasing their empathy (what it is like to be in the shoes of individuals labelled as challenging), and changing their attitudes. An outline of Who’s Challenging Who 2 Contact theory (the theory behind WCW) has already been used successfully to inform attitude change and reduce stigma towards other excluded groups, such as individuals with mental health difficulties.
For more information - http://www.mencap.org.uk/wales/projects/who-s-challenging-who"
Blogs on latest research findings from our team and comments on issues in the field of Intellectual Disability or Autism
Monday, 10 December 2012
Friday, 7 December 2012
Autism and Evidence 4. Does ABA work for children with autism?
This blog must be read after blogs 1-3 in
this series where I describe a way of thinking about “evidence”, and also where
I have explained what I think ABA is when applied to working in educational
contexts with children with autism. I am assuming that you have read these -
they are the context for the blog. Please don’t read the current blog out of
context!
The first thing to point out is that the
form of my question is just plain wrong. ABA is an applied science. So asking
if ABA “works” is like asking if Physics works or Ecological Science works. We
have to get more specific. The question needs to be posed about particular
ABA-based interventions. So, it is more appropriate to ask whether PECS works
for children with autism. It is also appropriate to ask whether Positive
Behaviour Support works in reducing behaviour problems in children with autism.
Thinking about some of our recent research, we have asked whether an Internet
reading programme based on ABA principles works for children with autism, and
whether using a buzzer prompting devise in a child’s pocket can help to improve
their social initiations towards peers.
In this blog, I do not intend to review
everything. For the moment, I will focus on whether a comprehensive ABA-based
educational model leads to good outcomes for children with autism. So, my
question might be about the evidence for Early Behavioural Intervention
(whether Intensive or not).
A little more
about my perspective
I also want to be clear about some of the
reasons I am writing this particular blog (and the series). I am not a
behaviour analyst. I am an academic psychologist and researcher working in the
field of intellectual disability (learning disability using the UK health
terminology) and autism. I have studied behaviour analysis and carried out a
good deal of research on ABA-based interventions for children with autism. However,
I am no spokesperson for ABA. I do hope that I
have a contribution to make as someone who has some understanding of ABA but
also more broadly psychology and perhaps policy/practice in the field of ID and
autism.
It is important to say that I am convinced
that there is something important in ABA interventions to support children with
autism. I am frustrated both by the “pro” and the “anti” ABA camps out there.
My opinion is that those pro ABA in the field of autism have been poor at
communicating their work and also sometimes poor at working with other autism
professionals and experts, including those who are autistic. I have to perhaps
include criticism of myself within this too. I am trying hard though, and this
blog is a part of that effort. Those anti ABA, in my opinion, have not taken
the time to fully inform themselves about ABA, fail to recognize the need to
specify the question about what ABA-based interventions might work, and are
preventing children and young people with autism benefiting from something
useful.
What am I “pro” then? I’m pro making a
positive difference to the lives of children and young people with autism. I
suspect anyone reading this blog will say the same thing. So, for goodness sake
let’s talk and collaborate and stop getting sidetracked away from the most
important agenda. Many children out there with autism even in the UK still
receive no specialized support, people do not understand their condition, and
when children receive some services there is poor practice (ABA and non-ABA
alike). Children and young people also face that cliff at transition to adult
services. The cliff is partly about even fewer services for adults, but also a
real lack of continuity from an educational focus to…err…what?
The evidence
for comprehensive Early Behavioural Intervention - a summary
My summary of the evidence can be represented
by this single picture that only makes sense in the context of my earlier
blogs:
Let me explain. A comprehensive ABA-based
educational model for young children with autism (early - so perhaps up to
around 7/8 years of age) is informed by basic scientific findings about
learning processes, and also by research suggesting that children with autism
can fail to develop typically during the early years. Comprehensive ABA models
are also informed by research describing how children develop typically, and by
autism research showing what skills early on might predict later positive
outcomes (e.g., joint attention and other social skills, communication skills).
These various aspects of scientific research have informed clearly described
comprehensive long-term educational programmes much of which is described in
manuals for interventionists. These interventions are conceptually rooted in
behavioural theory.
There are plenty of studies exploring
outcomes from the delivery of these described interventions, some of which
include comparison or control conditions. Lovaas’ (1987) seminal study might be
viewed as a part of this effort. Thus, Phases 0 and 1 in the evidence continuum
are covered pretty well.
I am not aware of a definitive randomized
controlled trial study yet published that was designed as a test of the
efficacy of comprehensive early behavioural intervention. That is why I have a
red cross at Phase 2 of the evidence continuum. Why is this? It is probably
safest to say that no-one really knows. There may be many reasons.
Historically, for example, when behaviour analysts were first delivering and
evaluating comprehensive ABA-based intervention for children with autism
(actually 30 years ago now or more) there may have been less clarity about the
importance of efficacy RCTs. Another problem has always been what the
comparison should be. As I pointed out in my third Blog in this series,
ABA-based methods are probably being used as a part of pretty much all
educational practice in the field of autism. So, how different might best
practice and comprehensive ABA actually look?
This touches on an ethical point some
researchers have made - that random allocation to an intervention lasting two
years or more is ethically complex when one already has some evidence that a
comprehensive ABA-based model may be beneficial. The control group could not
benefit from early intervention once that period has passed for them. So, that
possibility in their life would be lost. Others argue that RCTs of longer and
more intensive interventions in the field of autism can indeed be carried out.
There is no disagreement that this is possible (see Dawson Rogers et al ESDM
study mentioned in my second Blog in this series, for an example) - that RCTs
CAN be carried out. However, there is still an ethical debate to be had.
To be clear, the fact is that I am pretty
sure there is no knock-out efficacy RCT of comprehensive ABA-based educational
intervention for young children with autism. Commentators who point this out
are absolutely correct. We do not disagree. However, the point in describing
the evidence continuum to you is that we know this is not the whole story.
Anyone stopping at this point without providing the full picture is doing a
poor job of informing people about the evidence. The lack of an efficacy RCT is
perhaps a gaping hole, but it does not invalidate the remainder of the
evidence.
So, the full picture is that there are
quite a lot of Phase 3 outcome studies relating to comprehensive ABA-based
educational models. There are several effectiveness studies, including some use
of randomized designs. For example, Smith, Groen and Wynn in 2000 published a
paper where they had randomly allocated children to a comprehensive ABA-based
early intervention which was quite intensive and also delivered by experts, or
to an intervention where parents were trained in similar ABA skills (and they
delivered fewer intervention hours). The children in the expert delivery group
had better outcomes. There were still some positive outcomes for the parent
training group too. What I would take from this study is that these may be two
different delivery models for early behavioural comprehensive intervention that
can be clearly described. Both may be viable models depending on resources
available and family capacity, but if the studies results are replicated we
might conclude that an expert delivered model is likely to be more effective.
This sort of research could inform how intervention could be delivered on a
large scale.
Comprehensive
Early Behavioural Intervention - implementation research
What is pretty much unique in the field of
autism interventions is that there are also data at the implementation end of
the evidence continuum for ABA-based comprehensive early intervention. Several
groups in Canada, for example, have delivered large scale (i.e., hundreds of
children with autism) early behavioural intervention models and also evaluated
outcomes and published the results. Large scale delivery is not simple, and
several problems needed to be solved along the way including how to train
enough interventionists in the first place, and how to ensure that services
were delivering good quality intervention across large geographic areas. The
outcome studies are very encouraging in terms of making a difference to the
skills of young children with autism. Google (other search engines are
available!) the work of Adrienne Perry, Nancy
Freeman and colleagues in Ontario, and Isabel
Smith, Susan Bryson and colleagues in Nova Scotia.
We have also recently published two
implementation studies with colleagues in Wales [Grindle, C. F., Hastings, R.
P., Saville, M., Hughes, J. C., Huxley, K., Kovshoff, H., Griffith, G. M.,
Walker-Jones, E., Devonshire, K., & Remington, B. (2012). Outcomes of a behavioral education model for children with
autism in a
mainstream school setting. Behavior Modification, 36, 298-319], and in Norway [Eldevik, S., Hastings, R. P., Jahr, E., & Hughes, J. C. (2012). Outcomes of behavioral intervention for children with Autism in mainstream pre-school settings. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 210-220]. In both studies, we described how ABA comprehensive models can be delivered within existing services (mainstream pre-schools, and a special class within a mainstream infant school), and showed that outcomes were better than existing “education as usual” models.
mainstream school setting. Behavior Modification, 36, 298-319], and in Norway [Eldevik, S., Hastings, R. P., Jahr, E., & Hughes, J. C. (2012). Outcomes of behavioral intervention for children with Autism in mainstream pre-school settings. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 210-220]. In both studies, we described how ABA comprehensive models can be delivered within existing services (mainstream pre-schools, and a special class within a mainstream infant school), and showed that outcomes were better than existing “education as usual” models.
The main point here is that it would be a
mistake to evaluate these research studies as if they were designed to test the
efficacy of comprehensive ABA-based education for children with autism. They
should properly be considered as Phase 4 research - developing/describing and
evaluating the day-to-day delivery of comprehensive ABA intervention methods.
More
implementation research (practice-based evidence) needed
We cannot wait 20+ years for basic science
to inform new interventions and for these to go through the process of
gathering evidence right to the implementation stage. More evidence is needed
NOW to inform the design and development of educational services for children
with autism. It would also be premature to base these service design and
delivery discussions on evidence about interventions that have only reached the
stage of being tested for their efficacy (Phase 2). Such interventions are very
encouraging in terms of their outcomes, but we have no idea whether they will
prove effective and whether they will deliver good outcomes on a large scale.
Intriguingly, ABA-based intervention,
especially for young children with autism has been around so long that there
are examples and data around about successful implementation. Service providers
and commissioners must take notice of these unique data partly for what they
say about ABA-based interventions but also the lessons that might be learned in
relation to the delivery of other interventions.
Just as I advocated earlier in this blog
for pro and anti ABA camps to work together towards a common goal, researchers
and service providers need also to collaborate on implementation research. This
need is urgent. Educational services and professionals need to clearly define
their models and identify the evidence informing what they are trying to do.
What is the rationale for your educational model? Researchers then need to turn
to more applied research projects, devising ways to evaluate these defined
models. We can then start to have evidence-based educational service delivery
in the field of autism.
Thursday, 6 December 2012
Autism Evidence 3. What is ABA for children with autism?
Many people seem to misunderstand what
Applied Behavioural Analysis (ABA) actually is when applied to the education of
children with autism. I have found some limitations in understanding also among
ABA folks themselves. This blog is the result of many discussions with
colleagues about how to answer the question “What is ABA?” when applied to
working with children with autism. However, the description presented here is
my own perspective. Not all of my points will be agreed upon by all experts in
ABA. See whether you find this blog helpful.
What is ABA?
ABA has evolved from a science of learning
(called behaviour analysis). Basic scientific research has identified how
humans learn, and the application of this scientific knowledge for social
benefit is essentially what ABA is all about. ABA practitioners focus on
changing behaviour. “Behaviour” is not shorthand for problematic behaviours.
Although ABA practitioners do work with people to help reduce problematic
behaviours, much of the focus of ABA when working with children with autism is
on building new behaviours, new skills, and so helping children to learn new
things.
One of my colleagues is fond of saying that
whenever we achieve some change in behaviour, the principles of learning will
have been brought into play. The laws of learning are universal so whether we
intend to use them or not when people learn, when behaviour changes, this
happens via underlying learning processes.
Behavioural interventions, drawing on those
principles of learning and behaviour change, also feature in many contexts to
help improve quality of life. For example, in the UK, the National Institute for
Health and Clinical Excellence (NICE) has recommended a number of behavioural
interventions in different areas of health and well-being. These
recommendations include:
- Dialectical Behaviour Therapy for Borderline personality disorder (Jan 2009)
- Behavioural Activation for Depression in adults (Oct 2009)
- Multi-component intervention including behaviour change, and goal setting for Obesity (Dec 2006)
- Behavioural parent training in Parent training/education for child conduct disorder (July 2006)
- “Behavioural and functional analysis” for challenging behaviours in Dementia (Nov 2006)
- Behavioural intervention to improve daily living skills and for challenging behaviours in adults with autism (April 2012)
So, ABA is all around us, and is used in a
variety of contexts. In addition to ABA interventions in health, social care,
and education, ABA practitioners have used their skills to (amongst other
things): improve safety behaviours, train elite athletes, and to increase
productivity in work settings.
It has often been said that neither of the
As in ABA are for Autism. Hopefully, it is clear by now that ABA is not an
intervention for autism, but can be applied to help children with autism.
Defining ABA
for autism
The following single sentence might be a
good way to capture the essence of ABA when applied to working with children
with autism:
“ABA is a values-driven, child-centred,
developmentally-informed, evidence-based, effective use of principles of
learning to help children with autism achieve their full potential.”
The core values and commitments of ABA in
the field of autism are:
- ambition for the child, and optimism about what is possible for them
- an assumption of every child’s ability to learn (not dis-ability)
- empowering the child by establishing skills and supporting management of their own behaviour
- collaborative working with other professionals, family members, and carers to best support the child
A further characteristic that is important
to emphasise is that ABA does not “decide” on what behaviours to change or what
skills should be developed for a child with autism. ABA practitioners are
skilled at teaching skills and decreasing problematic behaviours, but the
behaviours to be changed need to be externally defined by others. When working
with children with autism, there are perhaps three sources used to identify the
foci for intervention:
- Children themselves, their families, and others around the child identify what outcomes are important to them
- An understanding of typical development (what key developmental skills has the child developed, or what they need to learn next), and also the requirements of statutory educational curricula
- A focus on pivotal behaviours that will facilitate further development: teaching communication, social skills, daily living or academic skills that can support independence and choice-making
Comprehensive
ABA intervention for children with autism
There are many ABA-based short-term focused
interventions carried out when working with children with autism. A particular
skill might be taught over a few days or weeks, or a particular behaviour
problem might be the target for reduction again over a few weeks. Behavioural
interventions are used probably in the majority of educational settings for
children with autism in this focused way.
The main debate in the field of autism
about ABA is actually focused on comprehensive ABA educational models. These
comprehensive models bring together multiple elements of ABA intervention in a
co-ordinated way and share probably all of the following features:
- Teaching multiple, rather than only one, skill areas
- Breaking down complex skills into small, teachable steps to facilitate learning
- Teaching over a long period of time – years, rather than weeks or months
- Can be used for all ages of children and adolescents
- The intervention model includes structured learning opportunities and also incorporates “natural” opportunities across much of the day/week
- Involves a team of people (often including parents)
- Can take place at home or in school and other settings
What may be a surprise to some is that I
have not included a prescription of intensity of intervention in this list of
shared features – no mention of the number of hours per week that must be
delivered. My perspective on this is that a focus on the number of hours of
intervention is linked to when comprehensive interventions are used as an early
intervention model. When used as an early intervention model, comprehensive ABA
intervention is often called Early Intensive Behavioural Intervention (or
EIBI). When intervening early, accepted wisdom is that one should deliver as
much intervention as possible. In fact, this is slightly more than accepted
wisdom. International expert consensus in the field of autism is that
intervention should ideally be early and the more that can be delivered
probably the better in terms of longer term outcomes. So, autism experts (not
ABA experts alone) recommend early and reasonably intensive intervention.
Again, I see no prescription of intensity
for a comprehensive ABA intervention. However, following international
consensus, one would want to deliver as much high quality intervention as
possible when delivering EIBI. If the resources available allow the delivery of
30-40 hours of intervention per week, one would want to do this. If the
resources allow for only 5 hours of intervention per week, this should (in my
view) be done too at a quality level as high as possible to ensure the biggest
impact for that input.
ABA and autism
after the early years
EIBI models have been developed to be used
early in the lives of children with autism. The focus is on behaviours and
learning that are developmentally appropriate for younger children. The
teaching methods used are suitable for young children. The outcomes of interest
are skills that might enable the child to benefit as much as possible from the
rest of their educational experience in school, ideally within an inclusive
educational setting. In fact, these are probably the general goals of all early
intervention models for children with autism not just ABA.
Wednesday, 5 December 2012
Autism evidence 2. Using an evidence tool to draw your own conclusions about autism interventions
Please read the first Blog in this series
[Who do you believe? A model for understanding the development of evidence for
interventions in autism] before reading this blog.
In practice, Thornicroft et al. (see model above) point out
that there are three points along the evidence continuum that they call blocks
– where evidence earlier in the storyline tends to get stuck. Many basic
science findings are not translated into interventions at all. This has often
been pointed out in the field of autism – there is a great deal of basic and
experimental research on children with autism, and much less use of this
research to inform new interventions. A second translational block is when
research demonstrates that an intervention CAN work, but then everyone relaxes
thinking that is enough to now get on and inform service delivery. Finally,
even when interventions are also tested in effectiveness studies research on
scaling up and delivering interventions within typical practice and service
settings is often just not done.
Using an evidence tool
By this point, I hope that you can see how
you could use Thornicroft et al.’s evidence tool. What it will hopefully prompt
you to do is to consider what question a particular research study is asking
about an intervention. The Phases of the model help to clarify the questions.
So, is someone describing to you an early study
designed to generate initial data on a recently defined new intervention (Phase
1)? If so, the research design might not be very strong or sophisticated.
However, the research is still likely to be important. You should be asking
about the theory underlying the intervention. The researchers or practitioners
should be able to articulate this clearly and point to the evidence that
supports their theory. A “good idea” is simply not enough – it has to be more
properly considered.
If you are being shown results from a
Randomized Controlled Trial, at what phase is this study along the continuum?
Is it an initial pilot trial (in which case it is not yet definitive evidence),
is it testing out whether an intervention CAN work, is it testing effectiveness
in more typical settings, or is it even using a RCT design to test aspects of
delivery (Phase 4) for an intervention with already established efficacy and
effectiveness?
In practice, you will find that the
evidence for interventions does not always follow the full continuum.
Sometimes, this will be because those peddling an intervention have not fully
understood what “evidence” really means. Sometimes, it suits those whose
livelihood relies partly on you being interested in a particular intervention
to give you only a part of the story. People may not be misleading you
explicitly – they are simply not presenting the whole story, or do not have a
way to easily communicate that full story to you. Now YOU have a way of looking
at evidence yourselves and can start to draw your own conclusions.
There are also good pragmatic reasons why
the development of evidence may not follow through the full set of phases.
First, this could take a very long time in total. It is often been pointed out
that the journey from basic science to practice can take two decades or more.
Thus, it is perfectly appropriate to take pragmatic decisions about what
evidence is most needed now. Obviously, we cannot sit around waiting for 20
years to develop evidence-based interventions for children with autism. We need
them now (or soon). This is not an excuse for poorly designed research.
However, researchers and practitioners could be more transparent about what
they are doing and why and could use the evidence tool to explain the bigger
picture and, therefore, the limitations of the evidence that they currently
have.
Some examples
At this point it might be helpful to show
how I might use the evidence tool to talk about the nature of evidence emerging
from individual intervention studies in the field of autism. I have chosen two
examples that use widely accepted strong research designs (both use RCTs), but
their focus is quite different.
Howlin and colleagues (2007) carried out an
evaluation of PECS (the Picture Exchange Communication System). 18 classes of
children with autism were allocated randomly to PECS or education as usual.
Teachers and parents received a 2 day PECS training workshop, and PECS trainers
then made 6 half-day consultation visits to each class over a 5 month period.
Although communication initiations and use of PECS increased in the PECS
intervention classes, there were no measured increases in scores on
standardized language assessments for the PECS group compared to the control
group. The increases in communication initiations and use of PECS also did not
continue after the consultation support ended.
I think the Howlin study of PECS is firmly
in Phase 3 of the evidence continuum. This is a study of the effectiveness of
PECS under typical conditions, delivered by school staff after minimal
training, with some supervision, in typical school settings for children with
autism. Questions that we might want to check up on, looking at this study on
its own, might be about the theory underlying PECS, whether there are efficacy
trials already published, and how could the intervention be scaled up and
included as a standard part of practice for children with autism in educational
settings (whilst also delivering outcomes)?
What is clear is that a study like Howlin’s
is simply a part of a story and is not the whole story.
Another recent and well-publicized study is
from Dawson, Rogers and colleagues (2010) evaluating the Early Start Denver
Model (ESDM). In their research, 48 young children with autism were allocated
randomly to EDSM or to “treatment as usual” for young children with autism in
their locality and received intervention over 2 years. The intervention was
delivered by highly trained and experienced graduate staff supervised by
experts. The research team also included the developers of the intervention
approach. The results were positive. For example, cognitive skills and adaptive
skills were improved in the ESDM group compared to the control group.
The Dawson and Rogers evaluation of EDSM
for me is a Phase 2 study – it is designed to test whether ESDM can work under
ideal and fairly tightly controlled circumstances. It was an efficacy trial.
The intervention is also based on basic developmental research in autism, is
manualized, and has been tested previously in other smaller scale studies (see
Phases 0 and 1). However, it would be a mistake to suddenly suggest that
everyone must start delivering ESDM as the model for pre-school services for
children with autism. Effectiveness studies are needed and, more significantly,
research is needed on how ESDM could be implemented as a part of a broader
pre-school educational service and on a large scale.
Again, the Dawson and Rogers study is very
important and a great piece of research, but it is only a part of the story.
A call to researchers and expert
practitioners
I would encourage researchers and expert
practitioners to use the evidence tool to summarize the evidence for particular
educational and psychological interventions for children with autism. To
properly critique any intervention, evaluate research evidence from basic
research findings right through to any effectiveness and implementation
studies. So, when you are explaining the evidence base for an intervention, see
if the evidence continuum tool helps you to provide the full story.
In a later blog, I will provide a
perspective on the evidence for Applied Behaviour Analysis intervention using
the evidence continuum tool.
Autism Evidence 1. Who do you believe? A model for understanding the development of evidence for interventions in autism
A continuum of evidence
All psycho-educational interventions for
children with autism are complex – they normally involve several components,
might target several outcomes, involve the child and other people, and will
typically occur in several environments and over a period of time. Several
people have pointed out that it can be helpful to think about a process for the
development of evidence for complex intervention that can be represented as a
continuum. Thornicroft, Lempp and Tansella (2011) described a model of the
process of development of evidence that I find very helpful
[see http://journals.cambridge.org/action/displayAbstract?fromPage=online&aid=8361312&fulltextType=ED&fileId=S0033291711000109]
I have re-drawn Thornicroft et al.’s mode above representing their phases of evidence development and emphasizing
increasing evidence from left to right.
An intervention should also be very clearly
defined or described (Phase 1) and ideally written down in detail in the form
of a manual for those using the intervention. The competencies needed to
deliver an intervention probably also need to be described and
training/supervision models developed. There would also need to be ways to
check whether someone is properly delivering an intervention (this is called
“fidelity”). A new intervention can then be subjected to initial testing –
trying it out, and measuring some outcomes. What outcomes to measure would also
be closely related to the underlying theory – what are you trying to change?
At some point during Phase 1 and in
preparation for Phase 2, we might also expect to see a Pilot Randomized
Controlled Trial (RCT) conducted. Using this research design, children with
autism would be randomly assigned to receive the intervention or to receive
another intervention (e.g., existing practice) or even no particular intervention
at all. A pilot RCT tests out everything ready to run a definitive RCT at Phase
2. Pilot RCTs are not designed to answer the question of whether an
intervention works.
During Phase 2 of the development of
evidence, research studies (probably large scale RCT studies) are carried out
to ask whether the intervention can work. This is a question of efficacy –
given well-resourced, well-trained and supervised interventionists delivering
an intervention with fidelity, CAN the intervention be shown to work? It is
very important at this stage to recognize that studies are not designed to tell
you whether an intervention will work when rolled out into practice.
Too often, the development of evidence in
the field of autism stops at Phase 2. Or at least, experts focus on this aspect
of evidence when they define what are evidence-based interventions. From
Thornicroft et al.’s model, you can see that this is far from the end of the
story. The next step at Phase 3 is to carry out tests of the effectiveness of an
intervention. CAN an intervention work is an important question, but whether it
still works in practice and under less ideal conditions is the question of
whether the intervention can be effective. Phase 3 research studies will often
be RCTs again. The focus, for example, might be on whether if you train people
who train others to deliver the intervention and they do this in typical school
settings using teachers or classroom assistants, do you still get positive
outcomes.
Tuesday, 2 October 2012
European Declaration on the Health of Children and Young People with Intellectual Disabilities and their Families
In some senses, this is old news. The World Health Organisation Europe led the process to develop the European Declaration on the Health of Children and Young People with Intellectual Disabilities and their Families, which was launched at a conference in November 2010.
See information at:
http://www.euro.who.int/en/what-we-do/health-topics/noncommunicable-diseases/mental-health/activities/intellectual-disabilities
The new dimension is that, finally, the Research Priorities document led by the International Association for the Scientific Study of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities has also been published to accompany the Better Health Better Lives report:
See information at:
http://www.euro.who.int/en/what-we-do/health-topics/noncommunicable-diseases/mental-health/activities/intellectual-disabilities
The new dimension is that, finally, the Research Priorities document led by the International Association for the Scientific Study of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities has also been published to accompany the Better Health Better Lives report:
http://www.euro.who.int/en/what-we-do/health-topics/noncommunicable-diseases/mental-health/publications/2012/better-health,-better-lives-research-priorities/_recache
The research priorities report can be downloaded for free.
The full reference is:
Emerson, E., Barron, D.A., Blacher, J., Brehmer, B., Clinch, S., Davidson, P.W., Davies, R., Felce, D., Glidden, L.M., Hassiotis, A., Hastings, R.P., Hatton, C., Heller, T., Holland, T., Janicki, M., Kerr, M., Knefel, M., Llewellyn, G., Murphy, G., Ouellette-Kuntz, H., Reinders, H., Timmons, V., Walsh, P.N., Weber, G. (2012). Better Health, Better Lives: Research Priorities. Copenhagen: World Health Organization Regional Office for Europe.
Thursday, 6 September 2012
Families research in the field of intellectual disability and autism
The International Association for the Scientific Study of Intellectual and Developmental Disability has published online a review of the state of the evidence on families of children with intellectual disability (learning disability in the UK) or autism. This is not a systematic review of all research evidence but more of a consensus process drawing on researchers internationally who work in the field.
There has been considerable interest in how families might be affected by having a child with significant disability in the family for some three decades or more. Family needs are important to understand so that the whole family is supported, and also that there is a positive family environment within which children with disabilities can develop.
The statement clearly identifies that although families may be under increased stress compared to other families, there are also significant positive experiences associated with raising a child with disability. These positive experiences can get over-looked.
Researchers are also clear in the statement about what we know very little about: the variety of family structures, families outside of Western societies, and what supports might work best to enable families to flourish and to continue to stay connected to their child with disability.
The document can be downloaded free as a pdf from the following website:
https://www.iassid.org/sirgs/families/documents-and-newsletters
There has been considerable interest in how families might be affected by having a child with significant disability in the family for some three decades or more. Family needs are important to understand so that the whole family is supported, and also that there is a positive family environment within which children with disabilities can develop.
The statement clearly identifies that although families may be under increased stress compared to other families, there are also significant positive experiences associated with raising a child with disability. These positive experiences can get over-looked.
Researchers are also clear in the statement about what we know very little about: the variety of family structures, families outside of Western societies, and what supports might work best to enable families to flourish and to continue to stay connected to their child with disability.
The document can be downloaded free as a pdf from the following website:
https://www.iassid.org/sirgs/families/documents-and-newsletters
Friday, 29 June 2012
Who’s Challenging Who? Changing the hearts and minds of support staff working with individuals whose behaviour challenges
See more info at
http://www.mencap.org.uk/wales/projects/who-s-challenging-who
Of course, there are also opportunities for further
research on the outcomes of WCW. In particular, a test of WCW against a control
condition is needed. Feedback from individuals attending the pilot training
also included the suggestion that WCW may be more powerful if co-delivered by
an individual from the services within which staff work. This model could be
explored in future research.
http://www.mencap.org.uk/wales/projects/who-s-challenging-who
Practice/Policy
context
Several UK care
scandals in the past two decades have identified that support staff working
with individuals with a learning disability whose behaviour challenges can
behave in ways that constitute abuse and maltreatment. The latest of these in
2011 focused on the BBC Panorama documentary about the Winterbourne View care
home.
The latest scandal
suggests that promises that “this can never happen again” have not been
fulfilled. This is despite appropriate policy and commissioning guidance, and
recommendations for best practice in working with people whose behaviour
challenges.
Professor Richard
Hastings (School of Psychology, Bangor University) has been carrying out
research for more than 20 years focused on staff who work with individuals
whose behaviour challenges. He argues that policies and guidance are too far
removed from the day-to-day factors that influence staff behaviour towards
those they support. Although Professor Hastings agrees that staff skills and
knowledge in understanding and intervening with challenging behaviour are
important, he also argues that two further dimensions are neglected. First,
support staff experience their work with challenging behaviour as emotionally
demanding. Second, staff attitudes towards individuals labelled as
“challenging” need to be changed. Change is needed to address these two
influences on support staff behaviour.
The Who’s
Challenging Who project
Who’s Challenging Who
(WCW) is an attitude change training package that has been developed over 18
months since January 2011 (pre-dating Winterbourne View). The development and
pilot evaluation of WCW was funded by a Knowledge Transfer Partnership grant between Mencap and Bangor University.
WCW involves a person with learning disability who has behaviour that
challenges working with a person without disability as co-trainers for a half
day workshop targeting health and social care support staff.
The WCW training
curriculum was informed jointly by the experiences of the co-trainers with
learning disability and by existing research evidence. The theory behind the
WCW training is to provide multiple opportunities for direct contact with
individuals whose behaviour challenges and contact with information about their
experiences of services and being labelled as “challenging”. Small groups of
support staff (up to 10) experience an intensive interactive session with the
aim of increasing their empathy (what it is like to be in the shoes of
individuals labelled as challenging), and changing their attitudes.
Contact theory (the
theory behind WCW) has already been used successfully to inform attitude change
and reduce stigma towards other excluded groups, such as individuals with
mental health difficulties.
Who’s Challenging
Who outcome data
Professor Hastings and
colleagues have recently carried out a research evaluation of the WCW training
package with 10 groups of staff (a total of 76 staff attended evaluation
groups). Research measures were used to assess staff attitudes towards those
whose behaviour challenges before the training course and after the WCW
training. Outcomes measured were: staff empathy, staff similarity attitudes
(beliefs about the similarity between people whose behaviour challenges and
other people), empowerment attitudes (beliefs about people whose behaviour
challenges being empowered to take control of their lives), and staff feelings
of confidence in supporting individuals whose behaviour challenges.
Statistically
significant changes pre-post WCW training were found for all four attitudinal
dimensions. Although statistical change is important because it suggests a
reliable positive change in the group of staff trained using WCW, it is also
important to ask how large the training effect was. Using metrics called effect
sizes, Professor Hastings and colleagues showed that the training effect was
“moderate” or “medium” in size. What this means is that the attitude changes
observed were clinically or practically meaningful, especially given that this
was only a half day training. Thus, the data on WCW effectiveness are very
encouraging.
What next?
WCW could be used as
an attitude change intervention in learning disability services throughout the
UK and internationally. WCW could be a part of the response to the Winterbourne
View scandal. Evidence-based approaches to changing staff attitudes within
services are sorely needed right now. WCW could be used in induction training,
as a refresher/booster training for staff who have worked in health and social
care for a long time, and/or alongside skills/knowledge-based training on
challenging behaviour.
Sexuality and young people with intellectual disability: Mothers' views
The final peer review scientific journal paper from our joint
research with Andrew Jahoda and Jaycee Pownall of Glasgow University, on
sexuality and young people with intellectual disability, has just been
published:
Pownall, J. D., Jahoda, A., & Hastings, R. P. (2012). Sexuality and sex education of adolescents with intellectual disabilities: Mothers’ attitudes, experiences, and support needs. Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 50, 140-154.
See link for abstract (summary of the paper)
http://aaiddjournals.org/doi/abs/10.1352/1934-9556-50.2.140
An easy read summary of the findings from the research can be downloaded at:
http://www.esrc.ac.uk/my-esrc/grants/RES-000-22-3124/outputs/Read/c8086eb2-d71c-4129-9bac-c931f7c359c7
A briefing paper for professionals and policy makers:
http://www.esrc.ac.uk/my-esrc/grants/RES-000-22-3124/outputs/Read/032e8627-8dde-460e-a996-4a37197aeb60
The overall aim of the research was to understand mothers' views about sexuality and their young adult children with intellectual disability. Some headline findings included that mothers of young adult children with intellectual disability:
Found it even harder to talk to their young adult children with ID about sexual matters than their other non-disabled children
Often believed that these young adults were not as interested in developing sexual relationships and had fewer sexual feelings
Believed they had fewer chances to have a girlfriend or boyfriend or to start a sexual relationship
Delayed talking about sex with them, as they felt it was less urgent
Found it more challenging to know when to talk about sex because their child did not ask questions about it
When sex was talked about, focussed on being clear about what is appropriate and inappropriate sexual behaviour
Found it hard to strike a balance between independence and protection
Felt there was a lack of information and support to help them address their children’s needs and tended to leave this up to schools/colleges
Felt they didn’t have enough knowledge about intellectual disability and its impact on their child’s sexuality
Were unsure if their child could understand information given to them about sex
Pownall, J. D., Jahoda, A., & Hastings, R. P. (2012). Sexuality and sex education of adolescents with intellectual disabilities: Mothers’ attitudes, experiences, and support needs. Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 50, 140-154.
See link for abstract (summary of the paper)
http://aaiddjournals.org/doi/abs/10.1352/1934-9556-50.2.140
An easy read summary of the findings from the research can be downloaded at:
http://www.esrc.ac.uk/my-esrc/grants/RES-000-22-3124/outputs/Read/c8086eb2-d71c-4129-9bac-c931f7c359c7
A briefing paper for professionals and policy makers:
http://www.esrc.ac.uk/my-esrc/grants/RES-000-22-3124/outputs/Read/032e8627-8dde-460e-a996-4a37197aeb60
The overall aim of the research was to understand mothers' views about sexuality and their young adult children with intellectual disability. Some headline findings included that mothers of young adult children with intellectual disability:
Found it even harder to talk to their young adult children with ID about sexual matters than their other non-disabled children
Often believed that these young adults were not as interested in developing sexual relationships and had fewer sexual feelings
Believed they had fewer chances to have a girlfriend or boyfriend or to start a sexual relationship
Delayed talking about sex with them, as they felt it was less urgent
Found it more challenging to know when to talk about sex because their child did not ask questions about it
When sex was talked about, focussed on being clear about what is appropriate and inappropriate sexual behaviour
Found it hard to strike a balance between independence and protection
Felt there was a lack of information and support to help them address their children’s needs and tended to leave this up to schools/colleges
Felt they didn’t have enough knowledge about intellectual disability and its impact on their child’s sexuality
Were unsure if their child could understand information given to them about sex
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)